This article mentions parts of:
Social learning from humans or conspecifics: differences and similarities between wolves and dogs
At present, beyond the fact that dogs can be easier socialized with humans than wolves, we know little about the motivational and cognitive effects of domestication. Despite this, it has been suggested that during domestication dogs have become socially more tolerant and attentive than wolves. These two characteristics are crucial for cooperation, and it has been argued that these changes allowed dogs to successfully live and work with humans. However, these domestication hypotheses have been put forward mainly based on dog-wolf differences reported in regard to their interactions with humans. Thus, it is possible that these differences reflect only an improved capability of dogs to accept humans as social partners instead of an increase of their general tolerance, attentiveness and cooperativeness. At the Wolf Science Center, in order to detangle these two explanations, we raise and keep dogs and wolves similarly socializing them with conspecifics and humans and then test them in interactions not just with humans but also conspecifics. When investigating attentiveness toward human and conspecific partners using different paradigms, we found that the wolves were at least as attentive as the dogs to their social partners and their actions. Based on these findings and the social ecology of wolves, we propose the Canine Cooperation Hypothesis suggesting that wolves are characterized with high social attentiveness and tolerance and are highly cooperative. This is in contrast with the implications of most domestication hypotheses about wolves. We argue, however, that these characteristics of wolves likely provided a good basis for the evolution of dog-human cooperation.
Cooperation is a fundamental aspect of human societies and has triggered much research in various fields like economics, psychology and biology. Although it is clear that human collaborative skills are exceptional, studying the cognitive and emotional processes of animal species that may underlie their cooperative interactions may reveal the evolutionary origins and the functional relevance of cooperation.
For a long time, the most common approach to investigate the evolutionary origin of human skills was to study non-human primates. More recently, building on the hypothesis that dogs and humans went through convergent evolution, researchers have suggested that dogs might be an additional, and in some respect, more informative model when investigating the evolution of human social behavior and cognition (Miklósi et al., 2004; Hare and Tomasello, 2005; Fitch et al., 2010). This idea is built on the assumption that dogs have been selected to cooperate and communicate with humans during domestication and, thus, evolved some genetic predispositions allowing them to develop skills shared with humans (Hare et al., 2002; Topál et al., 2009; Miklósi and Topál, 2013). Accordingly, it has been suggested that in a unique way, domestication has equipped dogs with two abilities necessary for cooperative problem solving—namely social tolerance and social attentiveness, that enable them to adjust their behavior to that of their social partners (Ostojić and Clayton, 2014).
Social tolerance, i.e., allowing a potential partner to come close even around food, has been shown to be a prerequisite of cooperation in several animal species. For instance, it has been argued that bonobos (Pan paniscus) outperform chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in cooperative interactions (Hare et al., 2007) because they are less aggressive and more tolerant in a food sharing context than their closest relatives, chimpanzees (Hare et al., 2007; Hare and Kwetuenda, 2010; see also Petit et al., 1992 on similar results in Tonkean (Macaca tonkeana) and Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta)). Moreover, also at an individual level, tolerant individuals usually outperform less tolerant ones in cooperative tasks (marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): Werdenich and Huber, 2002; chimpanzees: Chalmeau and Gallo, 1993; Melis et al., 2006b; rooks (Corvus frugilegus): Seed et al., 2008; Scheid and Noë, 2010). Along the lines of this argument, Hare and Tomasello (2005) proposed that selection for a tamer temperament and for reduced fear and aggression explains the higher success of dogs in cooperative and communicative interactions with humans in comparison to wolves, the closest wild-living relative of dogs (emotional reactivity hypothesis). Recently, this hypothesis has been extended to suggest that during domestication dogs became less aggressive and more tolerant than wolves not just toward humans but also toward conspecifics (Hare et al., 2012, for an extensive discussion of the two versions of the emotional reactivity hypothesis see Virányi and Range (2014)).
KEY CONCEPT 1. Social tolerance
Social tolerance refers to the close proximity of individuals (usually measured in the context of feeding), which is not accompanied with aggression or, if aggression occurs, it is bidirectional and ritualized. Across-species differences in social tolerance may rely on species-level differences in the underlying social emotions, but may also reflect more or less successful communication.
A second prerequisite of successful cooperation is social attentiveness, that is, paying sufficient attention to one's partners in order to adjust to their behavior and thus to cooperate (see for example studies using the loose string paradigm (Melis et al., 2006a,b; Seed et al., 2008; Péron et al., 2011; Plotnik et al., 2011)). Attentiveness toward potential partners, however, varies between species (Range et al., 2009a) and contexts (Range et al., 2009a) as well as according to the relationship the subject has with its partner (Range and Huber, 2007; Scheid et al., 2007; Horn et al., 2013). The link between attention and action coordination has so far mainly been studied in regard to social learning where several studies found that the amount of attention paid to potential models seems to be more or less directly linked to the success in acquiring specific behaviors (Lonsdorf, 2005; Ottoni et al., 2005; Renevey et al., 2013). Dogs have proven successful in several tasks that are thought to require high attention toward conspecifics and humans, such as experiments on social learning (Kubinyi et al., 2003; Topál et al., 2006; Range et al., 2007, 2011; Huber et al., 2009, 2014; Miller et al., 2009; Mersmann et al., 2011), social referencing (Merola et al., 2012a,b), communication (Virányi et al., 2004, 2006; Schwab and Huber, 2006; Udell and Wynne, 2008; Dorey et al., 2009; Kaminski et al., 2012), responding to unequal rewards (Range et al., 2009b, 2012) and cooperation (Naderi et al., 2001; Bräuer et al., 2013; Ostojić and Clayton, 2014). Furthermore, young dogs follow human pointing better and look at humans more readily than human-raised wolves (Miklósi et al., 2003; Gácsi et al., 2009). Consequently, it has been proposed that by means of positive (both evolutionary and ontogenetic) feedback processes, dogs have developed increased social attentiveness compared to wolves and thus, can achieve more complex forms of dog-human communication and cooperation than wolves (Miklósi et al., 2003; Virányi et al., 2008).
KEY CONCEPT 2. Social attentiveness
Social attentiveness describes to what extent an individual pays attention to its companions and monitors their behavior and interactions. Obviously, it can greatly facilitate one’s success in cooperating or competing with others or in gaining additional (and mostly highly relevant) information from their observation.
To sum up, these non-exclusive domestication hypotheses imply that wolves are less tolerant and less attentive than dogs. Importantly, however, all of the domestication hypotheses addressing the social skills of dogs and wolves rely on findings of experimental studies that compared the animals' interactions only with humans (Hare et al., 2002; Miklósi et al., 2003; Topál et al., 2005; Udell and Wynne, 2008; Virányi et al., 2008; Gácsi et al., 2009; Udell et al., 2011; Gácsi et la., 2013). Therefore, it is unclear if the few differences described so far reflect merely differences in the readiness of dogs and wolves to interact with humans or more fundamental differences regarding their social tolerance and social attentiveness; two prerequisites of cooperation. In the former case, we would expect that dogs and wolves show different behaviors only when interacting with humans but not conspecifics, simply because wolves are not quite as comfortable around humans as dogs and thus unable to fully concentrate on the human actions. In the latter case, however, we would expect dog-wolf differences also in other social contexts that do not involve humans. That is, in this case we would also expect differential social tolerance and/or attentiveness in interactions with conspecifics, as has been shown in other closely related species such as chimpanzees and bonobos or different macaque species as mentioned above.
KEY CONCEPT 3. Domestication hypotheses
Domestication hypotheses propose evolutionary scenarios to explain the behavioral differences of dogs and wolves. Importantly, the adaptational demands that have presumably shaped the social cognition of the domestic dog are thought to have played an important role also during human evolution. Many of these hypotheses imply that wolves are socially less tolerant and less attentive than dogs.
In order to better understand the effects of domestication on the social behavior of dogs, we think it is necessary to take a different approach, namely, rather than focusing purely on human-animal interactions, we should investigate to what extent and in what form the evolutionary precursors of dog social behavior can be found in wolves. To achieve this aim it is crucial to compare the interactions of dogs and wolves not only with humans, but also with conspecifics.
Obviously, living with or close to humans has certainly imposed important adaptational demands on the evolution of dog behavior (Miklósi and Topál, 2005; Topál et al., 2009); however, living in conspecific groups and interacting with other dogs has also always been part of the life of domestic dogs. Pet dogs represent a small part of the entire dog population with current estimates suggesting that free-ranging dogs represent about 76–83% of the global dog population (Hughes and Macdonald, 2013; Lord et al., 2013). These millions of dogs live more or less independently from humans, in conspecific groups in which their survival is greatly determined by successful communication and social maneuvering in intraspecific contexts (Bonanni and Cafazzo, 2014). Given this complex social ecology of the domestic dog, tracking the evolution of dog behavior and gaining a complete picture of the social competence of dogs and wolves require the study of various populations of both dogs and wolves as well as other canids that grew up and live in different social conditions (Udell et al., 2012; Miklósi and Topál, 2013).
Our contribution to this endeavor is to compare dogs and wolves that live in packs in large enclosures and are used to interacting with humans on a cooperative basis. For this aim, together with Kurt Kotrschal, we set up the Wolf Science Center that allows for testing adult wolves and dogs raised and kept in an identical way and socialized with humans and with conspecifics to a similar extent (Figure 1). Ensuring that wolves and dogs have the same experiences is important if we want to attribute observed dog-wolf differences to evolutionary changes rather than individual experiences. Pet dogs usually grow up in the human environment and thus have ample opportunities to learn how to interact and communicate with humans. On the one hand, these experiences can enhance their cognitive skills due to living in a much more complex social environment—an ontogenetic process that has been named “enculturation” (Call and Tomasello, 1996). On the other hand, having to interact and communicate with another species might greatly influence how attentive they are toward humans, which in turn affects their performance in social tasks involving humans (see also Miklósi et al., 2004; Topál et al., 2009). Thus, unless the animals grow up under identical conditions, not all dog-wolf dissimilarities should automatically be attributed to domestication, that is, assumed to rely on genetic changes that occurred since the dog separated from its closest wild-living relative, the wolf (Pang et al., 2009).
Figure 1. Life at the Wolf Science Center. Pictures show the socialization of the study animals with pet dogs (A), hand raising of the animals (B), interactions with humans (C), participation in experiments (D) and pack life of wolves (E) and dogs (F) (Photographers: F. Range, F. Schwärzler, H. Möslinger, W. Vorbeck, P. Kaut).
Our set-up is unique in comparison to earlier projects attempting to track evolutionary changes in canines (for an extensive review of these projects see (Virányi and Range, 2014)) in that (1) we can investigate not just the cognitive abilities of very young animals, but also of adult animals that have had similar experiences throughout their lives, (2) our set-up of keeping the animals in packs in large enclosures allows us to study their behavior with conspecifics under conditions that allow comparisons also with wild-living wolf and free-ranging dog packs, (3) their socialization enables us to explore the animals interactions with conspecifics as well as humans partly using the same experimental paradigms, and (4) we have an adequate sample size. At the Wolf Science Center we can study and compare human-animal as well as animal-animal interactions and can observe the animals' social behavior during spontaneous interactions within their packs.
So far, we have shown in several experiments that wolves pay as much attention to human partners as dogs do and that wolves can even outperform dogs in learning from observation of a conspecific, indicating the high social attentiveness of the species. We have also run experiments comparing their social tolerance toward conspecifics and humans, but this line of research is in progress and thus will not be discussed here in great detail.
Importantly, the results of our work so far have shown that the implications of the current domestication hypotheses about the low social attentiveness and tolerance of wolves are incorrect. Therefore, we have proposed the Canine Cooperation Hypothesis (Range and Virányi, 2013, 2014; Virányi and Range, 2014) that postulates that wolf-wolf cooperation constitutes the basis also for dog-human cooperation and that no additional selection for social attentiveness and tolerance was necessary to allow for dog-human cooperation to evolve (Figure 2). Rather, the latter has probably been facilitated by dogs becoming able to more easily lose their fear of humans and be comfortable around them, which is obvious in the less intensive socialization needed by dogs to avoid fear of humans in contrast to wolves (Scott and Fuller, 1965; Klinghammer and Goodmann, 1987). In this review, we will outline our results on the social attentiveness of dogs and wolves that support the Canine Cooperation Hypothesis as well as some previous data collected on social tolerance.
KEY CONCEPT 4. Canine Cooperation Hypothesis
Based on findings that in intraspecific contexts wolves are at least as socially attentive and tolerant as dogs, the Canine Cooperation Hypothesis postulates that dog-human cooperation evolved on the basis of wolf-wolf cooperation. In contrast to many domestication hypotheses, it suggests that dogs did not need to be selected for a general increase in their social attentiveness and tolerance.
Figure 2. Diagram of the “Canine Cooperation Hypothesis.”
To test whether wolves and dogs that have been socialized to humans to an equal amount, pay attention to human actions, we tested the WSC dogs and wolves in a local enhancement task, where a demonstrator indicated the location of a food reward. Local enhancement is thought to offer a plausible explanation as to how animals might learn where to find food by paying attention to a place or location where a conspecific is showing a species-specific behavior and subsequently visiting that place (Galef and Giraldeau, 2001; Renevey et al., 2013).
In our study, the subjects observed a familiar human (hand-raiser) (1) hide a food reward (1-day-old dead chick) in one of three possible locations on a meadow or (2) pretend to hide a food reward (Range and Virányi, 2013). The second condition was carried out to investigate how much attention the subjects paid to the details of the demonstration, i.e., if they recognized that food was actually hidden or not. Finally, in a third, control (no demonstration), condition the food reward had been hidden before the subject was led onto the meadow to test if the animals could find the food reward without any demonstration, only relying on olfaction. In this no demonstration control both wolves and dogs were less successful in finding the chick than in the first condition, where a human demonstrator hid the chick. Interestingly, the dogs outperformed the wolves in both test and control conditions, suggesting that they relied more on their nose to find the hidden food than the wolves. Furthermore, when comparing the “with” or “without” chick demonstration trials, we found no difference between wolves and dogs when a human was the demonstrator: both groups clearly differentiated whether or not the human demonstrator actually hid a chick or only pretended to do so (Figure 3A). These results show that wolves can use the information provided by a familiar human and pay sufficient attention to their actions to solve such a local enhancement task.
Figure 3. Social learning from a human and conspecific demonstrator. (A) depicts the success of wolves and dogs in the human demonstration (HUMAN_DEM) and respective control condition (HUMAN_CON). (B) shows the success of wolves and dogs in the conspecific demonstration (DOG_DEM) and control condition (DOG_CON). Adapted from Range and Virányi (2013).
Further support for this conclusion comes from another study where we investigated gaze following into distant space and around barriers in wolves (Range and Virányi, 2011). This ability to coordinate with others' head orientation to look in the same direction is considered a key step toward an understanding of others mental states like attention and intention (Baron-Cohen, 1995; Tomasello et al., 2005) and thus, is potentially also very important for being able to successfully cooperate. However, while gaze following into distant space could be simply a socially facilitated orientation response (i.e., a predisposition to look where others are looking) (Povinelli and Eddy, 1996), gaze following around barriers, where individuals need to reposition themselves to look behind the obstacle and assess the visual persepctive of the cue-giver different from their own, has been suggested to require a mental representation of the looker's visual perspective (Povinelli and Eddy, 1996) or learning how visual barriers impair perceptions (Tomasello et al., 1999). Accordingly, this latter ability to track another's gaze around obstacles seems to be cognitively more advanced, and has been suggested to occur especially in species with high levels of cooperative and competitive interactions (Schlögl et al., 2007). Our results showed that wolves followed human gaze as readily as conspecific gaze implying their high social attention and their readiness to accept humans as social partners who might provide important information.
However, the fact that wolves, when given intensive human socialization, accept humans as social partners and can use human-given information as well as dogs do, does not necessarily mean that domestication had no effect on dog-human cooperation. Instead, it is possible that domestication in dogs did alter these skills but in wolves intense socialization with humans provides an alternative route of acquiring them; for instance taking their natural fear of humans away may enable wolves to use their species-specific cognitive abilities also when interacting with humans (see Introduction, Udell et al., 2008).
Both social learning and gaze following are likely to be adaptive in wild-living wolves in several contexts (see for example, Thornton and Clutton-Brock, 2011). Wolves are cooperative breeders that rely on supporting each other not just when raising pups, but also in territorial defense and when hunting large game or defending their kills (Mech, 1970; Mech and Boitani, 2003; Kaczensky et al., 2005). This overall high dependency on cooperative interactions with conspecifics requires wolves to pay close attention to others in order to coordinate their actions with each other and probably some social learning to be able to learn the necessary skills for successful cooperation. For example, it has been proposed that wolf pups learn where to find prey, how to kill it and how to avoid injuries by accompanying their parents during hunting excursions and socially learning from them (Packard, 2003). Consequently, we propose that due to their social organization and their high dependency on cooperation, several emotional and cognitive characteristics that have been proposed to characterize dogs may already be present in wolves (Derr, 2011; Kotrschal, 2012; Range and Virányi, 2014). In other words, it is important to examine to what extent and in what format we can find the evolutionary precursors of dogs' social behavior in wolves. Since we have the best chance to detect such social capabilities of wolves in intraspecific contexts, we need to investigate the behavior of our wolves and dogs also in relation to conspecifics.
In the local enhancement study described in the previous section, we did not just test our animals with a human demonstration, but also with a dog demonstration. The subjects, both wolves and dogs, had established close relationships with the demonstrator dogs during the hand-raising period. They readily greeted them, played with them, and established dominance relationships with them, and all wolves and dogs readily submitted to the demonstrator dogs suggesting that they were perceived as conspecifics. In the test conditions, the subjects observed (1) the conspecific carrying and dropping a food reward (1-day-old dead chick) on one of the three paths, or (2) the conspecific walking out on a path without a food reward. These two conditions were interspersed with the three conditions mentioned above (human demonstrator hiding the food, human demonstrator pretending to hide food, control without demonstration) in an order counterbalanced across and within subjects. Interestingly, comparing the control condition with the food hiding demonstrations (human and conspecific), we found that both wolves and dogs benefitted from a demonstration independent of the demonstrator species. However, when comparing the “with” and “without” chick demonstration trials, we found a difference in contrast to the results with the human demonstrator: while the dogs showed the same behavior after a conspecific demonstration as after human demonstration, in general the wolves paid less attention to the dog in the “with” and “without” chick demonstrations than the dogs did, and wolves did not differentiate between these two trials (Figure 3).
These last results are intriguing, since the question arises why the wolves showed special attention to the human demonstrations and clearly differentiated between “with” and “without” chick demonstrations, but paid less attention to the conspecific demonstration independently of the presence of reward. There are two explanations that we think likely, although they need further testing: (1) Since our wolves have a very cooperative relationship with the hand-raisers, usually being rewarded for attention during the daily training sessions, they also expected to get food in these test trials. In contrast, the wolves did not expect the conspecific to share food with them and thus it was less interesting to pay close attention to them, or (2) although the demonstrator dogs were trained to execute the demonstration, they disliked the chick used as food reward. It is possible that the wolves recognized this dislike due to increased attention to the behavioral details of the conspecific models in comparison to the dogs. Assuming that wolves, similarly to tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) (Snowdon and Boe, 2003), are sensitive to a display of disgust and can adjust their behavior accordingly, the behavior of the demonstrators might have decreased the interest of the wolves in finding the food reward.
Both of these latter two speculations support the idea that wolves are very attentive toward behaviors of their social partners and maybe even more so than dogs. This idea is also supported by another social learning experiment, where we tested 6-months-old wolves and same aged dogs in a two-action imitation task following a conspecific demonstration (Range and Virányi, 2014). The subjects observed one of two familiar pet dogs opening a novel box using either its paw (Figure 4A) or its mouth (Figure 4B) to reach a food reward hidden inside. After each of six demonstrations, the observers were allowed to retrieve a piece of food from the box opened by the demonstrator. Afterwards, the subjects were released to manipulate the baited apparatus to see if and how they solved the task and whether they matched their behavior to the demonstrated action. We found that wolves clearly outperformed the dogs with all wolves opening the box at least once by actively manipulating the lever, while only 4 dogs out of the 15 were successful (Figure 4). Regarding imitation, we found that wolves significantly matched the demonstrated action when manipulating the box for the first time, whereas dogs used the two methods randomly. Moreover, wolves and dogs differed significantly from each other in regard to whether or not they matched the demonstrated action during their successful manipulation of the box with 9 of the 12 wolves matching the observed method, while none of the dogs did so.
Figure 4. Imitation of conspecifics. The pictures depict the two different demonstrations (A: Paw demonstration; B: Mouth demonstration) of opening the baited box, wolves and dogs observed six times before being allowed to try to open the box by themselves. The table lists the number of animals that first manipulated the most relevant part of the box, the lever, that were successful at least once in opening the box and finally used the method that they had observed to open the box. In all three variables the wolves and dogs differed significantly from each other (for further details see Range and Virányi, 2014).
It is possible that instead of trying to solve the problem by themselves, the dogs might have relied on the human experimenter to solve the task as has been shown in other experiments (Topál et al., 1997; Passalacqua et al., 2011; Horn et al., 2012) which might have masked their imitative abilities. However, our dogs first tried to open the box by themselves (we found no difference in the latency of wolves and dogs to manipulate the apparatus) and they approached a human only later, at a time when the wolves had already solved the problem, which suggests that this explanation is unlikely. Moreover, conducting control experiments, we could rule out that the difference between wolves and dogs arose due to differential developmental pathways of the two species or better physical insight of wolves. Rather it seems that the wolves were more sensitive toward the details of the action of a conspecific partner compared to the dogs, enabling them to solve this task.
To sum up, so far we have shown that wolves pay close attention to details of their pack-members' behavior when following their gaze or when imitating their action in a manipulative, two-action task. Moreover, since these animals are well-socialized with humans, they also follow human gaze into distant space, and profit from a human demonstration in a local enhancement task. Accordingly, wolves seem to possess at least one skill that has been suggested to be a precondition of successful cooperation, namely high social attentiveness toward social partners.
But how about the second skill proposed to be important for successful cooperation, social tolerance toward group members? Earlier observations of wolves and dogs raised and kept comparably, suggest that malamute puppies show earlier and more intense aggression than wolves (Frank and Frank, 1982). These observations were also confirmed in older animals, where dogs were overly aggressive in agonistic interactions and more so than wolves (Feddersen-Petersen, 1991, see Virányi and Range, 2014 for a thorough review of these data). In a first study investigating intra-species aggression and tolerance in our animals, we tested each animal with every other pack member in pair-wise food competition tests. We found that in dogs the higher-ranking member of each dyad monopolized the food resource, feeding alone most of the time and showed more aggression than the lower-ranking partner, whereas in wolves we did not find such an effect of rank position (Range et al. in revision). Moreover, the wolves co-fed longer than the dogs. In conjunction with Feddersen-Petersen's and Franks's observations, our data suggested that, at least in captivity, dogs form a steeper dominance hierarchy than wolves, which probably dissuades lower-ranking animals from challenging higher-ranking partners.
Similar comparisons have been conducted also in other closely related species, such as in macaques, for instance. Based on their aggression, tolerance, conciliatory behavior, dominance gradient and kin-bias, Thierry (2000) arranged the macaque species according to a four-grade scale. The first grade is characterized by unidirectional aggression of dominant animals with high and severe biting rates and subordinates generally fleeing or submitting when attacked. The species belonging here are characterized as having a steep dominance gradient and a low tolerance level. On the other extreme of the scale, the intensity of aggression and the biting rate are low, and most aggressive interactions are bidirectional, meaning that the victim of aggression protests or counter-attacks. In these species, the dominance gradient is less steep and tolerance is high. Thus, while the asymmetry of contests and the dominance gradient decrease from the first to the forth grade, social tolerance increases. Importantly, however, the dominance gradient is a characteristic of linear hierarchies and thus a low gradient does not imply that there is no linear and stable hierarchy. Since, as described earlier, the intraspecific social life of dogs has always been and is still highly relevant for the evolution of dog behavior, we suggest that comparing intraspecies aggression and tolerance in dogs and wolves can and should be integrated into such broader comparative and ecological frameworks. If adopting Thierry's (2000) scale to the dogs and wolves, the dog would be characterized as a less tolerant species than the wolf.
If attentiveness and tolerance are indeed relevant prerequisites for cooperation, we would expect that differences in those fundamental abilities will correlate with the degree of cooperativeness in various social contexts. As discussed earlier, the social life of wolves is centered on cooperation with their kin. In contrast to wolves, feral dogs do not live in family units but rather as multi-male/multi-female groups of largely unrelated individuals. And although they display differentiated social relationships with each other (Bonanni et al., 2010; Cafazzo et al., 2010), they do not breed cooperatively (Lord et al., 2013). Instead, female feral dogs mainly raise their pups alone (Daniels and Bekoff, 1989; Boitani and Ciucci, 1995) with fathers defending the pups occasionally. Moreover, dogs usually feed on stable food resources provided by humans (e.g., scavenging at rubbish dumps, or food provisioned by humans; Schmidt and Mech, 1997; Butler et al., 2004) and reports of group hunting in free-ranging dogs are rare (Butler et al., 2004; but see also Manor and Saltz, 2004). Consequently, dogs do not seem to be as cooperative as wolves with conspecifics, which has also been observed in captive settings (e.g., Feddersen-Petersen, 2007; but see Ostojić and Clayton, 2014).
In conclusion, our own and previous data support the Canine Cooperation Hypothesis (Range and Virányi, 2013, 2014; Virányi and Range, 2014) that argues that the high social attentiveness, tolerance and presumable cooperativeness in wolves provided a good basis for dog-human cooperation to evolve. Our results contradict most domestication hypotheses that argue that in comparison to wolves, dogs were selected for increased attentiveness and tolerance. The Canine Cooperation Hypothesis is, however, compatible with other evolutionary theories emphasizing the role that wolf-human similarities in sociability and cooperativeness played in the evolution of dogs (Schaller and Lowther, 1969; Clutton-Brock, 1984; Schleidt, 1998) and with the domestication hypotheses that specifically address the human-directed behavior of dogs. For instance, it has been suggested that dogs were selected for reduced fear of and easier socialization with humans (Scott and Fuller, 1965; Klinghammer and Goodmann, 1987); this certainly helped them to extend their relevant social skills to interactions with humans—skills inherited from wolves. Further research needs to clarify, however, whether indeed only this easier socialization with humans differentiates dogs and wolves, but otherwise dog-human and wolf-wolf cooperation rely on similar mechanisms or if domestication has resulted also in other changes in the dogs' behavior relevant to their interactions with humans.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The Wolf Science Center was established by Zsófia Virányi, Kurt Kotrschal and Friederike Range and we thank all our helpers who made this possible and thus indirectly supported this research. We further thank two anonymous reviewers, Ádam Miklósi and Rachel Dale for comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. The research described in this review has received funding by Austrian Science Fund (FWF) project P21244-B17, the European Research Council under the European Union's Seventh Framework Programme (FP/2007-2013)/ERC Grant Agreement n. [311870]' and the WWTF project CS11-026. ZsV was awarded also with the OTKA project K84036. We further thank many private sponsors including Royal Canin for financial support and the Game Park Ernstbrunn for hosting the Wolf Science Center. Friederike Range earned a Ph.D. in Psychology from the University of Pennsylvania in Philadelphia, USA, studying free-ranging non-human primates. Her postdoctoral research she conducted at the University of Vienna, where she, together with her colleagues, established the Clever Dog Lab and the Wolf Science Center. 2011 she got a position as senior researcher at the Messerli Research Institute at the University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna. Range's work on the effects of domestication on cognitive abilities of dogs is currently supported by an ERC starting grant. Zsófia Virányi received her Ph.D. on Ethology at the Eötvös University in Budapest, Hungary. She conducted postdoctoral projects at the Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research, Altenberg, Austria and at the University of Vienna. She is a founder of the Clever Dog Lab and the Wolf Science Center, and was the coordinator of the ESF Research Networking Programme “CompCog”. She holds a senior researcher position at the Messerli Research Institute at the University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna. Her work currently is supported by a WWTF and an FWF grant.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Boitani, L., and Ciucci, P. (1995). Comparative social ecology of feral dogs and wolves. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 7, 49–72. doi: 10.1080/08927014.1995.9522969
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bonanni, R., and Cafazzo, S. (2014). “The social organisation of a population of free-ranging dogs in a suburban area of Rome: a reassessment of the effects of domestication on dogs' behaviour,” in The Social Dog Behaviour and Cognition, eds J. Kaminski and S. Marshall-Pescini (Amsterdam: Academic Press), 65–104.
Bonanni, R., Cafazzo, S., Valsecchi, P., and Natoli, E. (2010). Effect of group size, dominance rank and social bonding on leadership behaviour in free-ranging dogs. Anim. Behav. 79, 981–991. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.02.021
CrossRef Full Text
Bräuer, J., Bös, M., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2013). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) coordinate their actions in a problem-solving task. Anim. Cogn. 16, 273–285. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0571-1
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Butler, J. R. A., Du Toit, J. T., and Bingham, J. (2004). Free-ranging domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) as predators and prey in rural Zimbabwe: threats of competition and disease to large wild carnivores. Biol. Conserv. 115, 369–378. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3207(03)00152-6
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Cafazzo, S., Valsecchi, P., Bonanni, R., and Natoli, E. (2010). Dominance in relation to age, sex and competitive contexts in a group of free-ranging domestic dogs. Behav. Ecol. 21, 443–455. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arq001
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (1996). “The effect of humans on the cognitive development of apes,” in Reaching into Thought, eds A. E. Russon, K. A. Bard, and S. T. Parker (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 371–403.
Google Scholar
Chalmeau, R., and Gallo, A. (1993). Social constraints determine what is learned in the chimpanzee. Behav. Processes 28, 173–179. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(93)90090-E
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Clutton-Brock, J. (1984). “Dog,” in Evolution of Domesticated Animals, ed I. L. Mason (London: Longman), 198–211.
Daniels, T. J., and Bekoff, M. (1989). Population and social biology of free-ranging dogs, Canis familiaris. J. Mammal. 70, 754–762. doi: 10.2307/1381709
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Derr, M. (2011). How the Wolf Became the Dog: From Wolves to Our Best Friends. New York, NY: The Overlook Press, 287.
Dorey, N. R., Udell, M. A. R., and Wynne, C. D. L. (2009). Breed differences in dogs sensitivity to human points: a meta-analysis. Behav. Processes 81, 409–415. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.03.011
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Feddersen-Petersen, D. U. (1991). The ontogeny of social play and agonistic behaviour in selected canid species. Bonn. Zool. Beitr. 42, 97–114.
Google Scholar
Feddersen-Petersen, D. U. (2007). “Social behaviour of dogs and related canids,” in The Behavioural Biology of Dogs, ed P. Jensen (Trowbridge: Cromwell Press), 105–119.
Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T., Huber, L., and Bugnyar, T. (2010). Social cognition and the evolution of language: constructing cognitive phylogenies. Neuron 65, 795–814. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.011
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Frank, H., and Frank, M. G. (1982). On the effects of domestication on canine social development and behavior. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 8, 507–525. doi: 10.1016/0304-3762(82)90215-2
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gácsi, M., Gyõri, B., Virányi, Z., Kubinyi, E., Range, F., Belényi, B., et al. (2009). Explaining dog wolf differences in utilizing human pointing gestures: selection for synergistic shifts in the development of some social skills. PLoS ONE 4:e6584. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006584
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gácsi, M., Vas, J., Topál, J., and Miklósi, Á. (2013). Wolves do not join the dance: sophisticated aggression control by adjusting to human social signals in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 145, 109–122. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2013.02.007
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Galef, B. G., and Giraldeau, L. A. (2001). Social influences on foraging in vertebrates: causal mechanisms and adaptive functions. Anim. Behav. 61, 3–15. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1557
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., and Tomasello, M. (2002). The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298, 1634–1636. doi: 10.1126/science.1072702
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hare, B., and Kwetuenda, S. (2010). Bonobos voluntarily share their own food with others. Curr. Biol. 20, 230. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.038
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hare, B., Melis, A. P., Woods, V., Hastings, S., and Wrangham, R. (2007). Tolerance allows bonobos to outperform chimpanzees on a cooperative task. Curr. Biol. 17, 619–623. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.040
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hare, B., and Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 439–444. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hare, B., Wobber, V., and Wrangham, R. (2012). The self-domestication hypothesis: evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Anim. Behav. 83, 573–585. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.007
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Horn, L., Range, F., and Huber, L. (2013). Dogs' attention towards humans depends on their relationship, not only on social familiarity. Anim. Cogn. 16, 435–443. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0584-9
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Horn, L., Virányi, Z., Miklósi, Á., Huber, L., and Range, F. (2012). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) flexibly adjust their human-directed behavior to the actions of their human partners in a problem situation. Anim. Cogn. 15, 57–71. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0432-3
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Huber, L., Range, F., and Virányi, Z. (2014). “Dog imitation and its possible origins,” in Domestic Dog Cognition and Behavior, ed A. Horowitz (Berlin; Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag), 79–100.
Google Scholar
Huber, L., Range, F., Voelkl, B., Szucsich, A., Virányi, Z., and Miklósi, Á. (2009). The evolution of imitation: what do the capacities of non-human animals tell us about the mechanisms of imitation? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 364, 2299–2309. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0060
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hughes, J., and Macdonald, D. W. (2013). A review of the interactions between free-roaming domestic dogs and wildlife. Biol. Conserv. 157, 341–351. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2012.07.005
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kaczensky, P., Hayes, R. D., and Promberger, C. (2005). Effect of raven Corvus corax scavenging on the kill rates of wolf Canis lupus packs. Wildl. Biol. 11, 101–108. doi: 10.2981/0909-6396(2005)11[101:EORCCS]2.0.CO;2
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., Schulz, L., and Tomasello, M. (2012). How dogs know when communication is intended for them. Dev. Sci. 15, 222–232. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01120.x
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Klinghammer, E., and Goodmann, P. A. (1987). “Socialization and management of wolves in captivity,” in Man and Wolf: Advances, Issues and Problems in Captive Wolf Research, ed H. Frank (Dordrecht: W. Junk Publishers), 31–60.
Kotrschal, K. (2012). Wolf – Hund – Mensch: Die Geschichte einer jahrtausendealten Beziehung, Brandstätter, 231.
Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., and Csányi, V. (2003). The effect of human demonstrator on the acquisition of a manipulative task. J. Comp. Psychol. 117, 156–165. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.117.2.156
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text
Lonsdorf, E. V. (2005). Sex differences in the development of termite-fishing skills in the wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Anim. Behav. 70, 673–683. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.12.014
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lord, K., Feinstein, M., Smith, B., and Coppinger, R. (2013). Variation in reproductive traits of members of the genus Canis with special attention to the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Behav. Processes 92, 131–142. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2012.10.009
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Manor, R., and Saltz, D. (2004). The impact of free-roaming dogs on gazelle kid/female ratio in a fragmented area. Biol. Conserv. 119, 231–236. doi: 10.1016/j.biocon.2003.11.005
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Mech, D. (1970). The Wolf: The Ecology and Behaviour of an Endangered Species. Garden City, NY: Natural History Press.
Mech, L. D., and Boitani, L. (2003). “Wolf social ecology,” in Wolves: Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation, eds L. D. Mech and L. Boitani (Chicago; London: The University of Chicago Press), 1–35.
Melis, A. P., Hare, B., and Tomasello, M. (2006a). Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators. Science 311, 1297–1300. doi: 10.1126/science.1123007
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Melis, A. P., Hare, B., and Tomasello, M. (2006b). Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees: tolerance constraints on cooperation. Anim. Behav. 72, 275–286. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.09.018
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., and Marshall-Pescini, S. (2012a). Dogs' social referencing towards owners and strangers. PLoS ONE 7:e47653. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047653
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Merola, I., Prato-Previde, E., and Marshall-Pescini, S. (2012b). Social referencing in dog-owner dyads? Anim. Cogn. 15, 175–185. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0443-0
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Mersmann, D., Tomasello, M., Call, J., Kaminski, J., and Taborsky, M. (2011). Simple mechanisms can explain social learning in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Ethology 117, 1–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01919.x
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Virányi, Z., and Csányi, V. (2003). A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do. Curr. Biol. 13, 763–766. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00263-X
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Miklósi, Á., and Topál, J. (2005). Is there a simple recipe for how to make friends? Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 463–464. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.009
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text
Miklósi, Á., and Topál, J. (2013). What does it take to become ‘best friends’? Evolutionary changes in canine social competence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 287–294. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.04.005
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Miklósi, A., Topál, J., and Csányi, V. (2004). Comparative social cognition: what can dogs teach us? Anim. Behav. 67, 995–1004. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.10.008
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Miller, H. C., Rayburn-Reeves, R., and Zentall, T. R. (2009). Imitation and emulation by dogs using a bidirectional control procedure. Behav. Processes 80, 109–114. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.09.011
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Naderi, S., Miklósi, Á., Dóka, A., and Csányi, V. (2001). Coopeartive interactions between blind persons and their dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 74, 59–80. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(01)00152-6
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ostojić, L., and Clayton, N. (2014). Behavioural coordination of dogs in a cooperative problem-solving task with a conspecific and a human partner. Anim. Cogn. 17, 445–459. doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0676-1
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ottoni, E. B., De Resende, B. D., and Izar, P. (2005). Watching the best nutcrackers: what capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) know about others' tool-using skills. Anim. Cogn. 8, 215–219. doi: 10.1007/s10071-004-0245-8
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Packard, J. M. (2003). “Wolf behavior: reproductive, social, and intelligent,” in Wolves: Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation, eds L. D. Mech and L. Boitani (Chicago; London: The University of Chcago Press), 35–66.
Google Scholar
Pang, J.-F., Kluetsch, C., Zou, X.-J., Zhang, A.-B., Luo, L.-Y., Angleby, H., et al. (2009). mtDNA data indicate a single origin for dogs south of yangtze river, less than 16,300 years ago, from numerous wolves. Mol. Biol. Evol. 26, 2849–2864. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msp195
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Passalacqua, C., Marshall-Pescini, S., Barnard, S., Lakatos, G., Valsecchi, P., and Previde, E. P. (2011). Human-directed gazing behaviour in puppies and adult dogs, Canis lupus familiaris. Anim. Behav. 82, 1043–1050. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.039
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Péron, F., Rat-Fischer, L., Lalot, M., Nagle, L., and Bovet, D. (2011). Cooperative problem solving in African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). Anim. Cogn. 14, 545–553. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0389-2
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Petit, O., Desportes, C., and Thierry, B. (1992). Differential probability of coproduction in two species of macaque (Macaca Tonkeana, M. Mulatta). Ethology 90, 107–120. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1992.tb00825.x
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Plotnik, J. M., Lair, R., Suphachoksahakun, W., and Waal, F. B. M. (2011). Elephants know when they need a helping trunk in a cooperative task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 5116–5121. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1101765108
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., and Eddy, T. J. (1996). Factors influencing young chimpanzees' (pan troglodytes) recognition of attention. J. Comp. Psychol. 110, 336–345. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.110.4.336
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Range, F., Horn, L., Bugnyar, T., Gajdon, G. K., and Huber, L. (2009a). Social attention in keas, dogs, and human children. Anim. Cogn. 12, 181–192. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0181-0
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Range, F., Horn, L., Virányi, Z., and Huber, L. (2009b). The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 340–345. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810957105
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Range, F., and Huber, L. (2007). Attention in common marmosets: implications for social-learning experiments. Anim. Behav. 73, 1033–1041. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.07.015
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Range, F., Huber, L., and Heyes, C. (2011). Automatic imitation in dogs. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 278, 211–217. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1142
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Range, F., Leitner, K., and Virányi, Z. (2012). The influence of the relationship and motivation on inequity aversion in dogs. Soc. Justice Res. 25, 170–194. doi: 10.1007/s11211-012-0155-x
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Range, F., and Virányi, Z. (2011). Development of gaze following abilities in wolves (Canis Lupus). PLoS ONE 6:e16888. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016888
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Range, F., and Virányi, Z. (2013). Social learning from humans or conspecifics: differences and similarities between wolves and dogs. Front. Psychol. 4:868. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00868
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Range, F., and Virányi, Z. (2014). Wolves are better imitators of conspecifics than dogs. PLoS ONE 9:e86559. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0086559
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Range, F., Virányi, Z., and Huber, L. (2007). Selective imitation in domestic dogs. Curr. Biol. 17, 868–872. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.04.026
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Renevey, N., Bshary, R., and van de Waal, E. (2013). Philopatric vervet monkey females are the focus of social attention rather independently of rank. Behaviour 150, 599–615. doi: 10.1163/1568539X-00003072
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Schaller, G. B., and Lowther, G. R. (1969). The relevance of carnivore behaviour to the study of early hominids. Southwest. J. Anthropol. 25, 307–341.
Google Scholar
Scheid, C., and Noë, R. (2010). The performance of rooks in a cooperative task depends on their temperament. Anim. Cogn. 13, 545–553. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0305-1
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Scheid, C., Range, F., and Bugnyar, T. (2007). When, what, and whom to watch? Quantifying attention in ravens (Corvus corax) and jackdaws (Corvus monedula). J. Comp. Psychol. 121, 380–386. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.121.4.380
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Schleidt, W. M. (1998). Is humaneness canine? Hum. Ethol. Bull. 13, 1–4.
Schlögl, C., Kotrschal, K., and Bugnyar, T. (2007). Gaze following in common ravens, Corvus corax: ontogeny and habituation. Anim. Behav. 74, 769–778. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.08.017
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Schmidt, P. A., and Mech, L. D. (1997). Wolf pack size and food acquisition. Am. Nat. 150, 513–517. doi: 10.1086/286079
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Schwab, C., and Huber, L. (2006). Obey or not obey? Dogs (Canis familiaris) behave differently in response to attentional states of their owners. J. Comp. Psychol. 120, 169–175. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.120.3.169
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Scott, J. P., and Fuller, J. L. (1965). Genetics and the Social Behavior of the Dog. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Google Scholar
Seed, A. M., Clayton, N. S., and Emery, N. J. (2008). Cooperative problem solving in rooks (Corvus frugilegus). Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 275, 1421–1429. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0111
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Snowdon, C. T., and Boe, C. Y. (2003). Social communication about unpalatable foods in tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). J. Comp. Psychol. 117, 142–148. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.117.2.142
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Thierry, B. (2000). “Covariation of conflict management patterns across macaque species,” in Natural Conflict Resolution, eds F. Aureli and F. B. M. De Waal (Berkeley, CA: University of California Press), 106–128.
Google Scholar
Thornton, A., and Clutton-Brock, T. (2011). Social learning and the development of individual and group behaviour in mammal societies. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 366, 978–987. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0312
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., and Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 675–735. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000129
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., and Agnetta, B. (1999). Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, follow gaze direction geometrically. Anim. Behav. 58, 769–777. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1192
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Topál, J., Byrne, R. W., Miklósi, A., and Csányi, V. (2006). Reproducing human actions and action sequences: “do as i do!” in a dog. Anim. Cogn. 9, 355–367. doi: 10.1007/s10071-006-0051-6
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á., Virányi, Z., Kubinyi, E., and Csányi, V. (2005). Attachment to humans: a comparative study on hand-reared wolves and differently socialized dog puppies. Anim. Behav. 70, 1367–1375. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.03.025
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., and Csányi, V. (1997). Dog-human relationship affects problem solving behavior in the dog. Anthrozoos 10, 214–224. doi: 10.2752/089279397787000987
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Topál, J., Miklósi, A., Gácsi, M., Dóka, A., Pongrácz, P., Kubinyi, E., et al. (2009). “The dog as a model for understanding human social behavior,” in Advances in the Study of Behavior, Vol. 39, eds H. J. Brockmann, T. J. Roper, M. Naguib, K. E. Wynne-Edwards, J. C. Mitani, and L. W. Simmons (Burlington: Academic Press), 71–116.
Google Scholar
Udell, M. A., Dorey, N. R., and Wynne, C. D. (2011). Can your dog read your mind? Understanding the causes of canine perspective taking. Learn. Behav. 39, 289–302. doi: 10.3758/s13420-011-0034-6
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., and Wynne, C. D. L. (2008). Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues. Anim. Behav. 76, 1767–1773. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.028
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Udell, M. A. R., Spencer, J. M., Dorey, N. R., and Wynne, C. D. L. (2012). Human-socialized wolves follow diverse human gestures… and they may not be alone. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 25, 97–117.
Google Scholar
Udell, M. A. R., and Wynne, C. D. L. (2008). Ontogeny and phylogeny: both are essential to human-sensitive behaviour in the genus Canis. Anim. Behav. 79, E9–E14. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.033
CrossRef Full Text
Virányi, Z., Gácsi, M., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Belényi, B., Ujfalussy, D., et al. (2008). Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves and dogs. Anim. Cogn. 11, 373–387. doi: 10.1007/s10071-007-0127-y
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Virányi, Z., and Range, F. (2014). “On the way to a better understanding of dog domestication: aggression and cooperativeness in dogs and wolves,” in The Social Dog. Behaviour and Cognition, eds J. Kaminski and S. Marshall-Pescini (Amsterdam: Academic Press), 35–62.
Google Scholar
Virányi, Z., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, A., and Csányi, V. (2004). Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans' attentional focus. Behav. Processes 66, 161–172. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2004.01.012
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Virányi, Z., Topál, J., Miklósi, A., and Csányi, V. (2006). A nonverbal test of knowledge attribution: a comparative study on dogs and children. Anim. Cogn. 9, 13–26. doi: 10.1007/s10071-005-0257-z
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Werdenich, D., and Huber, L. (2002). Social factors determine cooperation in marmosets. Anim. Behav. 64, 771–781. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2002.9001
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Keywords: domestication, evolution, cooperation, attention, Canis lupus, Canis familiaris, tolerance
Citation: Range F and Virányi Z (2015) Tracking the evolutionary origins of dog-human cooperation: the “Canine Cooperation Hypothesis”. Front. Psychol. 5:1582. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01582
Received: 24 July 2014; Accepted: 21 December 2014;
Published online: 15 January 2015.
Edited by:
Reviewed by:
Copyright © 2015 Range and Virányi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: friederike.range@vetmeduni.ac.at;
*Correspondence: zsofia.viranyi@vetmeduni.ac.at
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.